Elsevier

Behavioural Processes

Volume 117, August 2015, Pages 52-58
Behavioural Processes

Animal memory: A review of delayed matching-to-sample data

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.11.019Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We tested animal working memory using meta-analyses on delayed matching-to-sample data.

  • Without delays we found no evidence for differences between mammals, birds and bees.

  • We cannot exclude that birds match the performance exhibited by mammals.

  • Neither monkeys nor apes stand out when it comes to memory performance.

  • Animals use specialized and general memory systems for different kinds of information.

Abstract

We performed a meta-analysis of over 90 data sets from delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) studies with 25 species (birds, mammals, and bees). In DMTS, a sample stimulus is first presented and then removed. After a delay, two (or more) comparison stimuli are presented, and the subject is rewarded for choosing the one matching the sample. We used data on performance vs. delay length to estimate two parameters informative of working memory abilities: the maximum performance possible with no delay (comparison stimuli presented as soon as the sample is removed), and the rate of performance decay as the delay is lengthened (related to memory span). We conclude that there is little evidence that zero-delay performance varies between these species. There is evidence that pigeons do not perform as well as mammals at longer delay intervals. Pigeons, however, are the only extensively studied bird, and we cannot exclude that other birds may be able to bridge as long a delay as mammals. Extensive training may improve memory, although the data are open to other interpretations. Overall, DMTS studies suggest memory spans ranging from a few seconds to several minutes. We suggest that observations of animals exhibiting much longer memory spans (days to months) can be explained in terms of specialized memory systems that deal with specific, biologically significant information, such as food caches. Events that do not trigger these systems, on the other hand, appear to be remembered for only a short time.

This article is part of a Special Issue entitled: In Honor of Jerry Hogan.

Introduction

Animal memory is the object of enduring fascination and debate (Suddendorf and Busby, 2003, Raby et al., 2007, Clayton and Dickinson, 1998, Roberts, 2002, Bouton, 2007, Pearce, 2013, Kendrick et al., 1986). Even a cursory look at the literature reveals that sometimes animals appear to remember events in detail, and for a long time, while sometimes they seem to forget surprisingly fast. For example, many jays (family Corvidae) can remember the location of hidden food for many months (Bossema, 1979, Bednekoff et al., 1997), but have difficulty remembering simple color stimuli for more than 25–40 s (Olson et al., 1995). This is just one example of the great variation in memory performance that has led scholars to widely different conclusions. Some maintain that non-human animals have no explicit memories of past events, that is, they cannot recollect them in the same way as we do when we think about, say, childhood vacations (Roberts, 2002, Suddendorf and Busby, 2003). Others, in contrast, conclude that we have evidence that animals have “episodic-like” memories perhaps not dissimilar from those of humans (Clayton et al., 2001a, Clayton et al., 2001, Zentall, 2005). Comparison with human memory is central to the debate about non-human memory. Humans, in fact, appear capable of remembering practically arbitrary events for very long times without appreciable decline in performance, notably in tasks in which most other species perform poorly (Overman and Doty, 1980).

Here we review well-controlled experiments using the delayed matching-to-sample paradigm (DMTS, summarized below), one of a number of tasks that has been used to probe animals’ working memory (Bouton, 2007, Pearce, 2013). By “working memory” we mean information about an event that is maintained for some time in the absence of reinforcement, and that can be used to guide behavior at a later time (Pribram et al., 1960, Baddeley, 1991). We are particularly interested in events without specific biological significance, such as colored lights, visual patterns, or sounds, as typically used in laboratory studies. We ask whether we can discern any species differences in working memory, and whether memory can be improved by training. We conclude by suggesting that animal memory is best characterized as the sum of specialized memory capacities, different across species and potentially lasting a long time, and a general memory capacity that works in essentially the same way in all non-human species, and which has a limited span ranging between a few seconds and a few minutes.

Section snippets

The delayed matching-to-sample (DMTS) paradigm

In a typical DMTS experiment a sample stimulus is presented for a short time, typically a few seconds. The sample is then removed and, after a delay, two comparison stimuli are presented, of which one is identical to the sample and the other is different. The animal is rewarded for choosing the comparison stimulus that matches the sample (several procedural variations are possible, such as requiring the animal to perform an action to remove the sample and initiate the delay). This paradigm has

Results

All data reviewed are shown in Fig. 2, and Fig. 3 shows our estimates of zero-delay performance and performance half-life for the featured species. Our unit of analysis is a “data set,” i.e., data collected from the same individuals under the same conditions. Note that several data sets may come from the same individuals, tested more than once under different conditions. Dolphin data, for example, come from just two individuals, and capuchin data from four. In both cases, the animals were

Are there species differences in DMTS performance?

The main conclusion of our review is that many (perhaps all) non-human animals are capable of near-perfect performance in zero-delay match-to-sample, and that in all species performance degrades rather quickly when a non-zero delay is used, with good performance having been reported at intervals ranging from a few seconds to a few minutes. For comparison, Overman and Doty (1980) report that humans easily attain errorless performance after a 48 h delay.1

Acknowledgement

This study was supported by the Swedish Reseach Council.

References (81)

  • J.S. Rodriguez et al.

    CANTAB delayed matching to sample task preformance in juvenile baboons

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2011)
  • H.L. Sloan et al.

    Double dissociation between hippocampal and prefrontal lesions on an operant delayed matching task and a water maze reference memory task

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • T. Suddendorf et al.

    Mental time travel in animals?

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2003)
  • G.R. Wenger et al.

    Titrating matching-to-sample performance: effects of drugs of abuse and intertrial interval

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav.

    (1992)
  • T.R. Zentall

    Animals may not be stuck in time

    Learn. Motiv.

    (2005)
  • A. Baddeley

    Human Memory Theory and Practice

    (1991)
  • R. Berriman et al.

    Acquisition of delayed matching in the pigeon

    J. Exp. Anal. Behav.

    (1963)
  • Å. Björck

    Numerical Methods for Least Squares Problems

    (1996)
  • D.S. Blough

    Delayed matching in the pigeon

    J. Exp. Anal. Behav.

    (1959)
  • I. Bossema

    Jays and oaks: an eco-ethological study of a symbiosis

    Behaviour

    (1979)
  • M.E. Bouton

    Learning and Behavior: A Contemporary Synthesis

    (2007)
  • D.R. Brodbeck et al.

    Matching location and color of a compound stimulus: comparison of a food-storing and a nonstoring bird species

    J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process.

    (1995)
  • N.S. Clayton et al.

    Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays

    Nature

    (1998)
  • N.S. Clayton et al.

    Elements of episodic-like memory in animals

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci.

    (2001)
  • N.S. Clayton et al.

    Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) form integrated memories of the multiple features of caching episodes

    J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process.

    (2001)
  • Core Team, R.

    R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing

    (2013)
  • M.R. D’amato et al.

    Effect of delay-interval illumination on matching behavior in the capuchin monkey

    J. Exp. Anal. Behav.

    (1971)
  • L. Davachi et al.

    Primate rhinal cortex participates in both visual recognition and working memory tasks: functional mapping with 2-DG

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2001)
  • C.A. Dayer et al.

    Effects of ethanol on working memory and attention in pigeons

    J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.

    (2000)
  • M.R. Denny

    Retention of SR in the midst of the cognitive invasion

  • B. Diekamp et al.

    Nonspatial and subdivision-specific working memory deficits after selective lesions of the avian prefrontal cortex

    J. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • S.B. Dunnett et al.

    Proactive interference effects on short-term memory in rats: I. Basic parameters and drug effects

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1990)
  • M.R. Emmerling et al.

    PD 142676 (CI 1002), a novel anticholinesterase and muscarinic antagonist

    Mol. Neurobiol.

    (1994)
  • M. Etkin et al.

    Delayed matching-to-sample and short-term memory in the capuchin monkey

    J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.

    (1969)
  • D.P. Ferraro et al.

    Effects of chronic exposure to Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol on delayed matching-to-sample in chimpanzees

    Psychopharmacologia

    (1974)
  • G. Finch

    Delayed matching-from-sample and non-spatial delayed response in champanzees

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (1942)
  • J. Garcia et al.

    Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement

    Psychon. Sci.

    (1966)
  • J. Garcia et al.

    Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation

    Science

    (1955)
  • M. Giurfa et al.

    The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect

    Nature

    (2001)
  • K. Goto et al.

    Visual working memory of jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) in operant delayed matching-to-sample1

    Jap. Psychol. Res.

    (2009)
  • Cited by (70)

    • In search for consciousness in animals: Using working memory and voluntary attention as behavioral indicators

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Working memory processes become even more explicit when the DMS task is combined with distractor stimuli that actively needs to be suppressed (Jacob and Nieder, 2014; Jacob et al., 2018) or variable rule information the animal needs to integrate to successfully solve a task (Wallis et al., 2001; Bongard and Nieder, 2010; Eiselt and Nieder, 2013; Veit and Nieder, 2013). The DMS task has been used in many animal species from different taxa (Lind et al., 2015) and will be used here to identify the presence of working memory capabilities. Attention captures the selective processing of one aspect of the environment at the expense of others.

    • Effects of brief post-sample cues signaling presence or absence of reinforcers in delayed matching-to-sample

      2022, Behavioural Processes
      Citation Excerpt :

      The difference between the present results and those of past research may be related to differences in the duration of cue presentation. Stimuli that are more transient or temporally distant from outcomes that they signal generally exert weaker control over behavior, probably because they are forgotten over time (much like the sample stimulus exerts weak control at longer RIs in DMTS; see e.g., Cowie et al., 2011, 2017, 2020; Lind et al., 2015; White, 2001). In the present experiment, cues signaling reinforcer probabilities were presented for 0.5 s at the beginning of the RI.

    • On the working memory of humans and great apes: Strikingly similar or remarkably different?

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Nonetheless, Matsuzawa incorrectly refers to the performance of Ayumu as exhibiting a HWM size exceeding that of humans and that the performance of Ayumu is not matched by humans (Matsuzawa, 2013). However, according to a meta-analysis of > 90 sets of data taken from delayed match-to-sample studies covering 25 species from bees to birds and mammals, including primates (Lind et al., 2015), recall performance without delay following a stimulus varies little among species due to a ceiling effect of almost perfect performance by each species. It is therefore more informative to test different delay intervals when comparing WM across different species.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text